Attachment Informed Psychotherapy
Daniel Sonkin, Ph.D.
http://www.danielsonkin.com/
Thank you for your payment. Your transaction has been completed, and a receipt for your purchase has been emailed to you. You may log into your account at www.paypal.com/us to view details of this transaction.
To complete this class just read the material below, email me any questions you might have, and complete the quiz/evaluation (link below). Once I receive notification of your completion, I will send you an email with a link to your certificate of completion. I hope you find this class both interesting and helpful to your clinical practice. Bookmark this page for further reference.
Goals of Class
Rationale for Attachment
Theory
Who is an attachment
figure?
BowlbyÕs central
propositionÉ
É.that beginning in early
infancy, an innate component of the human mind -- called the Ņattachment
behavioral systemÓ -- in effect asks the question: Is there an attachment
figure sufficiently near, attentive and responsive?
If the answer is yesÉ..
Éthen certain emotions and
behaviors are triggered, such as playfulness, less inhibited, visibly happier
and more interested in exploration.
The "Strange
Situation" is a laboratory procedure used to assess infant/parent
attachment status. The procedure consists of eight episodes of separation and
reunion (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall, 1978). The infant's
behavior upon the parent's return is the basis for classifying the infant into
one of three attachment categories.
In the Strange Situation, secure infants are distressed when the parent leaves the
room. When the parent returns,
these infants are distressed (protest) but will quickly settle down and return
to playing and exploration.
If the answer is
consistently noÉ
Éa hierarchy of attachment
behaviors develop due to
increasing fear and anxiety
(visual checking; signaling to re-establish contact, calling, pleading; moving
to reestablish contact). If the
set of attachment behaviors repeatedly fails to reduce anxiety (get the
caregiver to respond appropriately) then the human mind seems capable of
deactivating or suppressing its attachment system, at least to some extent, and
defensively attain self reliance.
This leads to detachment.
In the strange situation,
these infants seem to be not phased by the parent leaving and disinterested
when the parent returns. But when
their heart rate is measured, they are indeed quite anxious. These infants are
classified as having an anxious-avoidant attachment to their attachment figure.
If the answer is
inconsistently noÉ
Éthe attachment behaviors
described previously become exaggerated as if intensity will get the attachment
figure to respond (which may or may not work). Like the dynamic between a
gambler and the slot machine, the attachment figure will pay off or respond in
sufficient frequency that the infant becomes preoccupied or anxious or
hypervigilant about the attachment figureÕs availability.
In the strange situation
these infants are very distressed when the parent leaves the room, canÕt settle
down after the parent leaves and canÕt settle down when the parent returns.
These infants are classified as having an anxious-ambivalent or resistant
attachment to their attachment figure.
Attachment disorganization
Originally researchers
described three categories (secure, anxious-avoidant and anxious-resistant) and
a final category termed Ņcan not classify.Ó Main and Solomon looked more closely at these unclassifiable
infants and found an interesting and consistent pattern that emerged. Some
children were particularly ambivalent upon reunion with their attachment
figure, both approaching and avoiding contact. Upon reunion some of these
infants would walk toward their parent and then collapse on the floor. Others
would go in circles and fall to the floor. Some would reach out while backing
away.
These infants appeared to
demonstrate a collapse in behavioral and attentional strategies for managing
attachment distress. They didnÕt display an organized strategy for coping with
attachment distress like the other categories (secure would cry and get
soothed, avoidant would ignore the parent, resistant would cling), so these
infants were termed, disorganized. Bowlby, in his book Attachment and Loss,
(1969) described some children in their caregiverÕs arms as "arching away
angrily while simultaneously seeking proximity.Ó
When researchers asked why
these children were both seeking protection from their caregivers while at the
same time pulling away, they discovered that a large percentage of these
infants were experiencing abuse by their caregiver. In other words, the person
who was supposed to be a haven of safety for the infant was also the source of
fear. Main and Hesse wrote that these infants were experiencing Ņfear without
solution.Ó
Another subgroup of
disorganized infants, however, were not experiencing abuse by their caregivers,
which the researchers found to be a curious anomaly. It was discovered that
these caregivers had experienced abuse by their parents, but that abuse was
still unresolved. It was discovered that when the infant was in need of
protection, the caregiver became frightened (may turn away or make subtle
frightening faces at the infant). It is believed that attachment
disorganization occurs when a parent acts either frightening or frightened in
response to the infants need for protection.
WhatÕs so great about
attachment security? Secure
children:
Cross Cultural Studies
The rates of attachment
patterns in both infants and adults are very consistent across cultures in
non-clinical samples (Main, 1990, Waters and Cummings, 2000). This would
make sense since attachment, from an ethological perspective, is biologically
based and handed down by evolution to promote survival of the species.
There has been criticism of BowlbyÕs theory as being inherently biased toward
western thinking (Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, Miyake, and Morelli, 2000), although
studies in non-western countries do show remarkable consistency with western
data. About 60-65% of the population is securely attached and about
35-40% are insecurely attached. The rates of insecure patterns in the US
samples are: 25% anxious-avoidant, 10% anxious-resistant and 5% disorganized.
In summary, the distribution of secure attachment classification in different
countries shows a striking similarity.
However, the rates of
insecure patterns are less consistent from culture to culture (van IJzendoorn
and Sagi, 1999). Differences have been attributed to the over-riding expression
of a cultural value, such as dependency or independence, and to differences in
perceived stress generated by the strange situation methods between
mother-infant dyads with different cultural experiences.
Attachment Terminology
Parent-Infant Attachment
Correspondence
The Adult Attachment
Interview is an evaluation tool to assess the attachment status of adults. It has been utilized to examine the
relationship between a parent's attachment status and the attachment
relationship between that parent and her/his infant (Main and Goldwyn, 1998) as
assessed in the Strange Situation. These studies have indicated that the
most robust predictor of the attachment pattern between the infant and her/his
parent is the attachment status of the parent. In other words, if a
parent has a secure state of mind of attachment, there is as high as an 80%
chance their infant will have a secure attachment to that parent. This is
true for insecure attachment as well. In other words, adults who are
securely attached are sensitive and cooperative parents therefore they will
engender these same qualities in their infants. Dismissive parents avoid
acknowledging their own attachment needs as well as those of their infant
and/or may be critical of their infants attachment needs therefore their
infants respond by minimizing their attachment needs and becoming avoidant.
Preoccupied parents respond to their childrenÕs attachment needs unpredictably
because they are still entangled in their own attachment experiences that
emotionally intrude in their present relationships. Their infants respond by
chronic attempts to feel secure and therefore, are clingy and difficult to
emotionally soothe. Disorganized parents are abusive or otherwise frightening
so their infants respond by approach - avoidance oscillation. These infants,
when they are needing protection from their caregiver, they simultaneously feel
fear and therefore, are experiencing Ņfear without solution.Ó
A meta-analysis was
conducted of 13 studies using three major categories. They found that:
A meta-analysis of 9 studies
using all four major categories found:
What does these data
suggest?
The attachment status (or
state of mind regarding attachment) of the parent, is going to have a direct
effect on the attachment of the infant to that parent - as high as 75%
predictability. In other words, secure adults engender security in their
children, dismissing adults tend to engender avoidant relationships with their
children, pre-occupied adults engender ambivalent attachment in their children
and adults with unresolved trauma or disorganization may act frightening or
confusing with their children, causing disorganized attachment in their
children.
Is attachment is a real
and separate phenomenon?
Temperament
Intelligence
Disability
Culture
Neurobiology of
attachment
Bowlby
believed that attachment was a biologically based behavioral system (Bowlby,
1989). However, it wasnÕt until the 1990Õs, the decade of the brain, with
the development of sophisticated scanning techniques that we were able to
literally look into the brain and better understand how this behavioral system
actually functions. The psychologist, Alan Schore, has brought together
findings from diverse areas such as clinical psychology, psychiatry, neurology,
developmental psychology and psychiatry to create a coherent understanding of
how the developing brain is impacted by attachment relationships
(1994).
There
is a rapid and significant brain growth spurt that occurs from the last
trimester of pregnancy through the second year. Infant MRI studies show that
the volume of the brain increases rapidly during the first 2 years. Most
importantly, imaging studies have indicated that the right hemisphere is
dominant in this early phase of development. A normal adult
appearance is seen by 2 years of age. All major fiber tracts are in place
by age 3 (Schore, 1994). Certainly the first two or three years of an
infantÕs life can be viewed as a time of opportunity, but may also be a time of
vulnerability.
According
to Schore, the important personality-creating experiences of parent-infant
attachment overlap with this period of brain growth spurt. He links the
right brain with self-regulation and the implicit self, which are shaped by
these attachment experiences (1994). He describes the right-brain to
right-brain communication that occurs between the caretaker and the infant as
being critical to the development of self-regulatory capacities.
Psychologist Peter Fonagy (2001), reiterates that attachment relationships are
formative because they facilitate the development of the brainÕs
self-regulatory mechanism, and that the enhancement of self/other emotion
regulation is key to healthy development.
What
are the mental capacities that are developing in the infantÕs brain during this
critical period? Siegel (1999) states early childhood experiences with
caretakers allows the brain (pre-frontal cortext in particular) to organize in
specific ways, which forms the basis for later interpersonal functioning.
Body maps, reflective function, empathy, response flexibility, social
cognition, autobiographical memory, emotion regulation are regulated in right
hemisphere. Clearly, a well-developed prefrontal cortext is critical to
experiencing healthy interpersonal relationships. Siegel (1999) states:
ŅIn
childhood, particularly the first two years of life, attachment relationships
help the immature brain use the mature functions of the parentÕs brain to
develop important capacities related to interpersonal functioning. The
infantÕs relationship with his/her attachment figures facilitates
experience-dependent neural pathways to develop, particularly in the frontal
lobes where the aforementioned capacities are wired into the developing brain.Ó
This
phenomenon, explains why there would be such a high correlation between a
parentÕs attachment status, as measured by the Adult Attachment Interview, and
the infantÕs attachment status, as measured by the Strange Situation.
Siegel (1999) goes on to say:
ŅWhen
caretakers are psychologically-able to provide sensitive parenting (e.g.
attunement to the infants signals and are able to soothe distress, as well as
amplify positive experiences), the child feels a haven of safety when in the
presence of their caretaker(s). Repeated positive experiences also become
encoded in the brain (implicitly in the early years and explicitly as the child
gets older) as mental models or schemata of attachment, which serve to help the
child feel an internal sense of what John Bowlby called Ņa secure baseÓ in the
world. These positive mental models of self and others are carried into other
relationships as the child matures.Ó
Clearly,
the neurobiology literature has opened the door to our developing a deeper
understanding of the attachment behavioral system and itÕs correlates in the
brain. Bowlby would have been not amazed by these newer developments, but
would have felt validated that his innovative theory has been substantiated by
so many researchers and embraced by clinicians. Many clinicians
still wonder why these neurobiological findings are so significant. It is
not enough to know that a client may have insecure attachment, but that moving
from insecure attachment to secure is in reality effecting changes in brain
function. It is critical that clinicians understand that insecure
attachment is not just an intellectual concept, but that it relates to specific
patterns of brain function and that it can be deconstructed to specific
capacities of the right prefrontal cortext that significantly impact a persons
interpersonal functioning – affect regulation, empathy, response
flexibility, knowing how your body is responding to a emotionally competent
stimulus and the ability to identify feelings, to name a few. Most
clinicians will agree that these are important capacities that one must possess
to successfully avoid many of the affect regulation problems people experience
in their relationships. Therefore, we are not just involved in changing
behavior, but helping our clients develop important neural capacities, that
they may be deficient in because of early childhood experiences.
There is another important
reason why the neurobiology findings are critical to therapists. The
techniques we typically utilize to effect change in treatment such as
interpretation, education, and skill building may not be sufficient to bring
about lasting (one may even say – neurobiological) change in our
clients. Schore suggests (2003a; 2003b) that the right-brain to
right-brain attunement that occurs between a parent and infant is primarily a
non-verbal, non-intellectual process. He suggests that psychotherapists
must appreciate this fact if they want to make an impact on the
neural-capacities of the right brain. This is similar to cross-cultural
counseling, but the different culture we are trying to understand is in the
right hemisphere of our client. The right hemisphere processes information
quite differently from the left hemisphere (Trevarthen, 1996). The right
hemispheres specialization in affective awareness, expression and perception,
which should be interesting to clinicians who are helping people learn to
develop more healthy ways of functioning in these areas.
However, the language of the right hemisphere is different from the left.
As opposed to the left hemisphere, whose linguistic processing and use of
syllogistic reasoning (looking for logical, linear cause-effect relationships)
which we are so used to utilizing in our day to day living, the language of the
right hemisphere is non-verbal and body-oriented (Siegel, 2001). It would
make sense that changing these capacities of right-prefrontal functioning, will
necessarily involve a non-verbal and body-awareness component. One of my
recommendations of this class will be to encourage therapists to utilize their
non-verbal and bodily reactions in psychotherapy to better understand their
clients and ultimately help them understand themselves and develop more
adaptive affect regulatory capacities. We will explore the pragmatics of
this process further when we discuss the therapeutic process.
Adult Attachment
In
the 1980s, the field of adult attachment began to evolve. This occurred
for several reasons. First, many attachment labs were conducting research
on the continuity of attachment status over time. Researchers were also
becoming interested in the long-term effects of secure and insecure attachment
on interpersonal functioning (Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, and
Albersheim, 2000). As the research in child, adolescent and adult
attachment evolved, new methods of assessing attachment status were
needed. Mary Main and her colleagues (Main and Goldwyn, 1993) at the
University of California, Berkeley developed the Adult Attachment Interview
(AAI). The interview has been utilized in hundreds of studies world wide
to assess adult attachment states of mind. The adult attachment literature
utilizes somewhat different category terminology. Each adult term
corresponds to an infant term. - secure, dismissing (anxious-avoidant infants),
preoccupied (anxious-resistant infants) and disorganized or unresolved
(disorganized infants).
In
longitudinal studies, children assessed in the strange situation as infants are
administered the AAI as young adults to determine the continuity of attachment
patterns over time (Waters, Hamilton, and Weinfield, 2000). According to
these studies there is about an 80% continuity between infant attachment patterns
and adult attachment state of mind (Fraley, 2002). In 20% of the cases
the attachment status changes over time (usually from insecure to secure, but
sometimes the other way). The term Ņearned securityÓ is used for those
individuals who were either assessed in the strange situation as insecure and
later in life are assessed as secure, or whose experiences in childhood would
ordinarily lead us to expect an insecure state of mind (strange situation data
is not available) but are assessed as secure on the AAI (Roisman, Padron,
Sroufe and Egeland, 2002). This category of Ņearned secureÓ is
significant for clinicians, because it suggests that attachment status is
changeable. In other words, how a child or adult regulates attachment
distress can change over time. What factors contribute to earned
security? Researchers (Roisman, Padron, Sroufe and Egeland, 2002) have
found that when a child changes from insecure to secure, it is most likely to
be affected by a relationship. This makes sense because insecurity grows
out of relationships, so one would expect Ņearned securityÓ to grow out of
relationships.
Another
important way the AAI data has been utilized is to examine the relationship
between the parent's attachment status and the attachment relationship between
that parent and her/his infant (Main and Goldwyn, 1998). These studies
have indicated that the most robust predictor of the attachment pattern between
the infant and her/his parent is the attachment status of the parent. In
other words, if a parent has a secure state of mind of attachment, there is as
high as an 80% chance their infant will have a secure attachment to that
parent. This is true for insecure attachment as well. In other
words, adults who are securely attached are sensitive and cooperative parents
therefore they will engender these same qualities in their infants.
Dismissive parents avoid acknowledging their own attachment needs as well as
those of their infant and/or may be critical of their infants attachment needs
therefore their infants respond by minimizing their attachment needs and
becoming avoidant. Preoccupied parents respond to their childrenÕs
attachment needs unpredictably because they are still entangled in their own
attachment experiences that emotionally intrude in their present relationships.
Their infants respond by chronic attempts to feel secure and therefore, are
clingy and difficult to emotionally soothe. Disorganized parents are abusive or
otherwise frightening so their infants respond by approach - avoidance
oscillation. These infants, when they are needing protection from their
caregiver, they simultaneously feel fear and therefore, are experiencing Ņfear
without solution.Ó
During
the 1980s, social psychologists also became interested in attachment in adult
relationships and itÕs relationship to interpersonal and group processes.
Out of this track came a large body of social-psychological research on
attachment style (rather than
attachment status, the term used
by developmental psychologists) and interpersonal functioning. Social
psychologists developed their own self-report measures of attachment that could
be quickly administered to a larger group of subjects and can scored relatively
easily. Attachment was deconstructed differently, depending on the research
group. For example, Shaver and colleagues view attachment patterns as
existing on two continuums, anxiety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark and Shaver,
1998). Low anxiety and low avoidance characterizes secure
attachment. Dismissing attachment is characterized by low anxiety
and high avoidance. Preoccupied attachment is characterized by high
anxiety and low avoidance. And disorganized attachment is characterized
by high anxiety and high avoidance.
Bartholomew
and her colleagues have deconstructed attachment more in line with BowlbyÕs
initial conceptualization – internal working models of self and others
(Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991). Like Shaver and his colleagues,
Bartholomew places attachment on two continuums – negative and positive
feelings about self, and negative and positive feelings about others.
Secure individuals have positive feelings about self and others.
Dismissing individuals have positive feelings about self, but negative feelings
about others. Preoccupied individuals have positive feelings about
others, but negative feelings about self. And disorganized individuals
have negative feelings about self and others. Although there was some initial
conflict between the self-report measures and interview methods, recent studies
has suggested that these different assessment tools may have more consistency
than originally thought (Shaver, Belsky and Brennan, 2000).
A
number of important findings have emerged from the research on
attachment. Attachment is a form of dyadic emotion regulation (Sroufe,
1995). Infants are not capable of regulating their own emotions and
arousal and therefore require the assistance of their caregiver in this
process. How the infant ultimately learns how to regulate his/her
emotions will depend heavily on how the caregiver(s) regulates his/her own
emotions. As children become better at expressing their needs and
emotions, they learn self-regulation skills. However, this dyadic
regulation never entirely disappears. There is a time for both types of regulation
(self and dyadic) throughout a person's life.
Another
important finding is that attachment is not a one-way street. As the
caregiver affects the infant, the infant also affects the caregiver. This
process is referred to as "mutual regulation" (Tronick, 1989).
The "attunement" of the caregiver is critical to secure attachment
patterns (Stern, 1985). Parents who are sensitive to the verbal and
non-verbal cues of the child are able to experience the infant in their mind
(hold the infantÕs mind in their mind), and are more likely to have securely
attached infants. This is referred to as mentalizing ability or
reflective function – that ability to hold the infants mind in their mind
(Fonagy, Target, Gergely and Jurist, 2002). For the majority of securely
attached individuals, the positive and adaptive manner in which they have
learned to modulate attachment distress, learned through their interactions
with their caregivers early in life, will continue unless their circumstances
change or other experiences intervene. Likewise, with insecure
infants and children, their particular behavioral coping mechanisms (of
avoidance, resistance or approach/avoidance) may become more behaviorally
sophisticated, but the net result (over-activating or under-activating) will
essentially continue as the individual ages. Research has documented that
adults assessed as having an insecure state-of-mind or insecure attachment
style with regard to attachment have greater difficulties in managing the
vicissitudes of life generally, and interpersonal relationships specifically,
than those assessed as securely attached (Shaver and Mikulincer, 2002).
Mary Ainsworth highlighted
the function of the attachment behavior system in adult life, suggesting that a
secure attachment relationship will facilitate functioning and competence
outside of the relationship.
ÓThere is a seeking to
obtain an experience of security and comfort in the relationship with the
partner. If and when such security and comfort are available,
the individual is able to move off from the secure base provided by the
partner, with the confidence to engage in other activities."
Adult Attachment
Development (Shaver and Clark, 1994)
Secure adults have mastered
the complexities of close relationships sufficiently well to allow them to
explore and play without needing to keep vigilant watch over their attachment
figure, and without needing to protect themselves from their attachment figures
insensitive or rejecting behaviors.
Preoccupied: What begins with attempts to keep track of or hold
onto an unreliable caretaker during infancy leads to an attempt to hold onto
partners, but this is done in ways that frequently backfire and produce more
hurt feelings, anger and insecurity.
Dismissing: What begins with an attempt to regulate attachment
behavior in relation to a primary caregiver who does not provide, contact,
comfort or soothes distress, becomes defensive self-reliance, cool and distant
relations with partners, and cool or hostile relationships with peers.
Unresolved/Disorganized/Fearful: What begins with conflicted, disorganized,
disoriented behavior in relation to a frightening caregiver, may translate into
desperate, ineffective attempts to regulate attachment anxiety through approach
and avoidance.
Insecure Attachment &
Psychopathology
Insecure attachment is not
the same as psychopathology, rather it is thought that insecurity creates the
risk of psychological and interpersonal problems (Sroufe, 2000). Although some clinicians find the idea
of classification of attachment status as similar to diagnosis (categorizing
and itÕs inherent limitations), the assessment of attachment status is a
completely different paradigm and process of classification so clinicians
should not use the categories in the same way as one would use a psychiatric
diagnosis.
Assessing Attachment
Status
There
are two general methods for assessing attachment in adults, interview methods
and self-report scales. The most common interview method is the Adult
Attachment Interview (AAI) developed by Mary Main and her colleagues at the
University of California at Berkeley (Main and Goldwyn, 1993).
The
Adult Attachment Interview
The
Adult Attachment Interview contains 20-questions that asks the subject about
his/her experiences with parents and other attachment figures, significant
losses and trauma and if relevant, experiences with their own
children. The interview
takes approximately 60-90 minutes. It is then transcribed and scored by a
trained person (two weeks of intensive training followed by 18 months of
reliability testing). The scoring process is quite complicated, generally but
it involves assessing the coherence of the subject's narrative. Mary Main
describes a coherent interview in the following way.
"...a
coherent interview is both believable and true to the listener; in a coherent
interview, the events and affects intrinsic to early relationships are conveyed
without distortion, contradiction or derailment of discourse. The subject
collaborates with the interviewer, clarifying his or her meaning, and working
to make sure he or she is understood. Such a subject is thinking as the interview proceeds, and is
aware of thinking with and communicating to another; thus coherence and
collaboration are inherently inter-twined and interrelated" (Slade, 1999,
page 580).
Some
sample questions from the AAI are:
1. I'd like you to choose five adjectives that reflect
your childhood relationship with your mother. This might take some time, and
then I'm going to ask you why you chose them. (Repeated for father)
2. To which parent did you feel closest and why? Why
isn't there this feeling with the other parent?
3. When you were upset as a child, what would you do?
4. What is the first time you remember being separated
from your parents? How did you and they respond?
What
is it about the coherence of a life story that reflects the attachment status
of the subject? There are differing ideas for this, but what seems
like the most plausible explanation is, when telling one's life story, it is
likely to generate subtle and not so subtle emotions about those experiences.
How well one is able to identify and regulate their emotions is going to, in
part, determine the way the story is told. Reading the transcripts of
securely attached individuals, their stories are coherent in the manner Main
described above. Dismissing adults tend to have extremely brief
stories. Many don't recall
memories of childhood. Those
who have untoward experiences either deny their occurrence or rationalize their
negative feelings and claim that those experiences made them stronger and more
independent. Preoccupied
individuals tend to get caught up in negative, analytic discussions of their
past and therefore their transcripts tend to be excessively long. Their
past tends to intrude on their present discussions of attachment and can be
extremely devaluing or idealizing of their attachment figures. Their
narratives are entangled and hard to follow. Disorganized individuals
tend to have lapses in the monitoring of reasoning and discourse in their
interview when discussing loss or experiences with abuse (Hesse, 1999).
The AAI protocol is available at the Stony Brook Attachment Lab web site
at:
http:
//www.psychology.sunysb.edu/attachment/measures/measures_index.html
Sample answers to the
AAI: Secure
Sample answers to the
AAI: Dismissing
Sample answers to the
AAI: Preoccupied
Reflective
Function
Another method similar to
the AAI was developed by Peter Fonagy and Mary Target of the Psychoanalysis
Unit of University College, London. They use the AAI questions, but the transcript is analyzed
from from the perspective of Ņreflective function.Ó Scoring the narrative involves
assessing the speaker's ability to reflect on their own inner experience, and
at the same time, reflect on the mind of others (Fonagy and Target, 1997). This mentalizing ability is thought to
be what secure parents do to imbue security in their children. Fonagy writes
that reflective function is a cognitive process - how an individual understand
the self and others intentions, needs, motivations. It is also an emotional process - the capacity to hold,
regulate, and fully experience emotion. A person with high reflective function
exhibits a non-defensive, willingness to engage emotionally, to make meaning of
feelings and internal experiences without becoming overwhelmed or shutting
down. From a neurobiological
perspective, high reflective function includes neural capacities such as social
cognition, autonoetic consciousness, awareness of and regulation of complex
emotional states inherent in social relationships – all capacities of the
prefrontal cortex.
ŅA motherÕs capacity to
reflect upon and understand her childÕs internal experience is what accounts
for the relation between attachment status and her childÕs sense of security and safety.Ó (Slade, 2002).
Adult
Attachment Projective
Another
promising method of assessing adult attachment is the Adult Attachment
Projective (AAP) developed
by Carol George of Mills College, and Malcolm West of the University of Calgary
(George and West, 2001). The test consists of eight drawings (one neutral scene
and seven scenes of attachment situations). According to the authors, "the
drawings were carefully selected from a large pool of pictures drawn from such
diverse sources as children's literature, psychology text books, and
photography anthologies. The AAP drawings depict events that, according to
theory, activate attachment, for example, illness, solitude, separation, and
abuse. The drawings contain only sufficient detail to identify an event;
strong facial expressions and other potentially biasing details are absent. The
characters depicted in the drawings are culturally and gender representative" (page 31).
Like
the AAI, the subject's responses are recorded and transcribed and then scored
based on the coherence of the responses. Authors use some similar and
different scales from the AAI coding process. According to the authors the AAP takes less time to
administer and much less time to score, which makes it more useful for
clinicians. Unlike the AAI, the AAP is geared toward clinicians as
opposed to only researchers in attachment. For more information see their
web site at: http:
//www.attachmentprojective.com/.
Self
report scales
The
other method of assessing adult attachment is with self-report scales.
The Experiences in Close Relationships Scale: Revised, developed by Phillip
Shaver and his colleagues (Brennan, Clark and Shaver, 1998), is a self report
scale that measures attachment security on two dimensions, anxiety and
avoidance. The first scale developed had three questions. Since then, it has been expanded to 36
questions. Their most recent version was based on a scale developed by
Kim Bartholomew: the Relationship Status Questionnaire (Bartholomew and
Horowitz, 1991). Because
they have many of the same items, these two scales correlate highly with one
another (Shaver, Belsky and Brennan, 2000). One important difference between
their two scales is in how they deconstruct attachment. Shaver and his colleagues
view attachment on two continuums, anxiety and avoidance. How an
individual scores on each of these subscales will determine their attachment
classification. Bartholomew, on the other hand, deconstructs attachment
also on two continuums: working models of self and others (either positive or
negative) (Bartholomew and Moretti, 2002). Her approach was more in line
with Bowlby's initial cognitive conceptualization of attachment. However,
what these two tests reveal is that the cognitive (Relationship Status
Questionnaire) and emotional/behavioral (Experiences in Close Relationships
Questionnaire) dimensions are all linked with regard to attachment. The
advantage of these self-report scales is that they are easy to administer and
score, and therefore clinicians do not need special training in their use.
All
of Shaver's scales can be accessed at the UC Davis Attachment Lab web site at; http: //psyweb2.ucdavis.edu/labs/Shaver/. In fact, Shaver has an online version of his
Experiences in Close Relationships scale that therapists and clients could take
and then receive their results immediately. This can be found at: http: //www.yourpersonality.net.
Bartholomew's
scales can be accessed at her web site at: http://www.sfu.ca/psyc/faculty/bartholomew/research/index.htm.
Assessing attachment
categories via the clinical interview
Can attachment status be
assessed via a clinical interview? Unfortunately, clinicians are
not as accurate as they would like to think they are. And the studies of
comparing clinician's diagnostic abilities and psychometric testing support
this contention. But
it is my belief that as a clinician gets to know his/her client's over time,
and carefully observe their behaviors and listen to their language, attachment
patterns begin to emerge and can be clearly recognizable. However, this
takes time and good observation on behalf of the clinician. So in the
meantime, using one of the available methods of assessing attachment status is
worthwhile.
Tasks of
attachment-informed psychotherapy according to Bowlby (1988)
Other neurobiological
considerations
**Autonoetic, autobiographical or extended consciousness is a form of episodic memory (the remembering of past experiences). Whereas noetic consciousness is the knowing of facts (my father beat me), autonoetic consiousness involves having a sense of self at the time (my father beat me and I felt so angry at him and I still feel that way today).
It is believed that this form of memory is mediated by the pre-frontal cortext and hippocampus (based on brain damage and imaging studies).
Knowing the self over time is very central to most psychodynamic therapies. When I ask my patient, "Why do you think you get so angry at your partner when he withdraws?", I am checking to see if they have this autonoetic consciousness ability. Consider these possible responses to this question.
1. "It just upsets me whenever he does that."
2. "I guess it's been going on for years with my partner and I think I tend to over-react when he does that."
3. "My father left us when I was very young. I think it caused me tremendous pain and anger. When my partner withdraws and feel rejcted, but I tend to go a bit overboard because of my experience with my father. I guess I am sensitive to being left."
I think the differences are obvious here. The awareness doesn't change the fact that the partner withdraws, nor does it stop our patient from feeling hurt or sad or angy when it happens. But this level of consciousness does have the potential of tempering the reaction. It puts the current event into a larger autobiographical context. I think developing this perspective is an important part of psychotherapy. Ultimately it's a more adaptive form of affect regulation. The Adult Attachment Interview is largely assessing this capacity. The more autonoetic consciousness, the more likely the subject will tell their story in a coherent manner.
What are emotions?
(Damasio, 1999)
Emotionally competent stimulus
Emotion process
What are feelings? (Damasio,
1999)
Brain asymmetry and
intervention
Another
exciting concept in the affective neurosciences is the notion that different
parts of the brain specialize in different capacities. Daniel Siegel
(1999) writes extensively about the notion of neural integration and how
integrated systems respond more flexibly and adaptively to problem situations.
Neuro-imaging technology has made it become increasingly clear that the
different hemispheres of the brain (right and left), even of the same
neuro-structures may have different functions. Richard Davidson (2004)
has found differences in the patterns of activation of the prefrontal cortex
with regard to approach and avoidance emotions. His studies have included
brain scans of monks who have studied with the Dali Lama (Davidson,
2000). He found that these individuals had particularly positive outlooks
on life and this was reflected by difference in the activation of their right
and left prefrontal cortex. Individuals who have an overall positive
outlook on life, are more likely to have higher left to right prefrontal
activation in response to problem solving, as compared to individuals who have
a more negativistic outlook on life (who have a lower left to right ratio of
activation). In other words, some people do really see the glass as half
full and others really see it as half empty. What is most interesting
about his work is that the pattern of activation can be changed through
mindfulness techniques.
Individuals
with secure attachment are likely to have this more positive outlook, whereas
individuals with insecure attachment are more likely to possess a negative
outlook. This data suggests that perhaps an important part of
psychotherapy may include teaching certain clients mindfulness techniques in
the service of developing more effective affect regulation strategies. If
emotion begins in the body, then training the mind (the prefrontal cortex in
particular) to be more mindful of the body and itÕs changes will help a person
be more aware of their emotions. My clinical experience has indicated
that patients with moderate to severe affective disorders who participate in
meditation and other similar practices report that these activities
dramatically increase feelings of wellbeing, and when practiced consistently,
and can have a long-lasting effect.
Mikulincer
(Mikulincer, Gillath, and Shaver, 2002) have found that perceived threats will
activate the attachment behavioral system and that adults with insecure
attachment will respond in the ways they have learned to cope in the past,
either hyperactivating or under deactivating the system depending on the attachment
style.
What
these findings suggest, is that the regulation of affect, particularly with
individuals with insecure attachment, is much more complex than early theories
of intervention have suggested. That learning to identify and tolerate
both negative and positive emotional states involves understanding what an
emotionally competent stimulus is, how the wide range of types of emotions are
activated in the body, and how consciousness is necessary to allow the
individual to feel the emotion and make adaptive choices with regard to
responding to the stimulus. Most importantly, the notion that the final
goal of this complex process is to achieve a state of well-being, rather than
simply neutrality or some resting state of quiescence, is one reward for the
change in the strategies in the first place. The other reward is to have
a more positive and mutually gratifying interpersonal relationships.
How this relates to
attachment-informed psychotherapy?
Addressing Unresolved
Loss and Trauma: Clinicial considerations
Case vignettes
Vignette #1: Robert
34 year old African-American
Started therapy shortly
after a separation from a 14 year marriage.
No children.
CPA for a bank.
Wife reports that he
smothered her, in that he was excessively jealous, dependent and verbally
abusive. Also states that he
refused to have children.
Robert presents as very
friendly, talkative and anxious.
He seems interested in my ideas and asks me on numerous occasions, ŅWhat
do I think he should do to get his wife back?Ó When asked about his childhood experiences, he launches into
a tirade about his fatherÕs unavailability (he worked three jobs to support the
family) and his motherÕs involvement with other men. He goes on for ten minutes and then stops and says, ŅI donÕt
know if that answers your question.Ó
At this point in the
interview I am feeling a bit overwhelmed by his anxiety. He goes on to say that he has never
found anyone as committed as he is in relationships - even friends are
unreliable. There is a long pause
and then he says, ŅYou know, people are never there when you need them.Ó
He explains, ŅIf Elaine
loved me more and was committed to being a family, I wouldnÕt be here in the
first place.Ó
When I ask about other
problems in the marriage he states that sex was also problem. He stated, ŅShe never seemed
interested. ŅWe hardly had
sex.Ó When I inquire as to frequency he replies Ņ..four or five
times a week.Ó
I take a deep breath and go
on asking about the jealousy. When
I ask if he thinks that his jealousy about his wife may be related to his
experiences in his family growing up he says, ŅI never thought about that.Ó
When asked about how he is
feeling recently since the separation, he states, ŅIÕve been sending her
flowers and emails apologizing for anything I can think of, but she wonÕt
forgive me.Ó
Robert expressed some
insight that his jealous feelings are not founded in reality (that his wife was
not with other men), but when she worked or went out with friends or even when
she was on the phone, he felt these intense feelings and believed if he could
get her attention he wouldnÕt feel so bad. This insight represented an open door that Robert might be
able to focus on himself long enough to make use of therapy.
Robert: Assessment
Robert: Treatment
Preoccupied individuals have
learned to become hypervigilant regarding their attachment figures. They are used to hyperactivating their
attachment distress in order to stay connected or get their attachment figureÕs
attention. Robert will need to:
These dynamics also came up
in the therapy. I take quite a bit
of time off each year (usually 8-10 weeks), so I was able to use the natural
ruptures that occur in sessions as opportunities for growth and change as
well. Initially, Robert had
trouble leaving the sessions on time.
He would always bring up a new topic at the end of the session. Endings were particularly difficult for
him. For the few two or three
years of therapy, Robert would announce his wanting to quit after my two week
or four week vacations. During the
later, during the first two or three years he would have continuity sessions
with his psychiatrist (Robert was on a very low dose of SSRI to manage his
anxiety and depression).
Currently he is back with
his wife (they actually reconciled after about a year separation). She has also been in therapy and they
have decided to explore the possibility of having a child. Initially, I think Robert initially
agreed to this out of desparation, but as he is learning to self-regulate, he
needs that exclusive relationship with his wife less to calm his
anxieties. So I think he is
genuinely open to the idea of being a father. Having not had a close relationship with his own father, our
relationship has been a model for him.
In summary, working with
Robert has been about helping him learn to self-regulate rathr than using
proximity maintenance with his wife.
This dynamic became apparent in our relationship too, so I was able to
use the natural ruptures that occur in our sessions (beginning and ending the
sessions, holidays, vacations, illnesses, etc.) to help him find more adaptive
ways of coping with the intense feelings of vulnerability that these events evoke
in him (emotionally competent stimuli).
By connecting with his body and labeling those experiences (core
consciousness) he was able to learn a new language of communication. He also learned how his past
experiences would intrude on his current experiences (implicit memory) and
developed an ability to connect past, present and future (extended
consciousness) and therefore have the ability to make a choice in his response
to the situation (response flexibility).
Robert has also recently gotten involved with medition, which I believe
has allowed him to wean himself from his medication which are at
sub-therapeutic doses at this time.
Vignette #2: Carolyn
32 years old – English
decent
Has been married for six
years and has a 18 month old daughter.
Works full time as an
accountant.
They have a full time nanny
living with them.
Carolyn came into therapy
because she has been dissatisfied with her marriage for the past two
years. She describes her husband
as immature and enmeshed with his family.
She states that she is constantly reminding her husband to do things
saying, ŅHeÕd forget to go to work each morning if I didnÕt remind
him.Ó She generally presents as cold and critical. She doesnÕt seem to want closeness with
her husband, just that he be more responsible and less dependent on her.
Ever since the birth of
their child, she feels constantly irritated at him, is not sexually attracted
to him. Says that her husband is
self-absorbed, controlling and not responsive to her needs. When I ask what needs she is
referring to she discusses help with the baby and taking care of the
house. When I ask about her
emotional needs she asks, ŅWhat do you mean?Ó
T: What was your parentÕs relationship
like?
C: It was ok.
T: Well, how would you describe it?
C: They were close.
T: Could you tell me a memory that
illustrates how they were close?
C: Well, letÕs see. I donÕt remember specific details, just
kind of images.
T: Images are ok.
C: We were on vacation
once. We used to drive up to
Wisconsin during the summers.
T: Uh huh.
C: They would sit there in the front seat
of the carÉmy mom would be reading and my dad would be listening to music on
the radio.
T: What was your relationship with your
parents like as a child?
C: I donÕt really remember when I was
really youngÉbut I donÕt think most kids want to tell their parents what they
are really thinking. I mean, why
give them that power, then they will have an advantage over you. No, itÕs better to just be quiet.
I learn later that her
parents divorced while she was a junior in high school. When I asked her why she thought they
got divorced she said she really didnÕt know. I asked her how the divorce affected her.
C: It was good for me
because I became more independent.
Besides, I was able to get away with murder.
T: What do you mean?
C: Well, they didnÕt really communicate
with each other, so I would play one off the other.
T: I see.
C: So they both ended up not knowing what
I was really up to.
T: Ok.
C: Not that they could
have stopped me. It was just
better that they didnÕt know what I was up to.
T: And the more
independent part, what do you mean about that?
C: They just didnÕt focus on me so much
after the divorce so I think it was a good thingÉI guess.
Later in the session I ask
about her current relationship with her parents.
T: What is your relationship with them
now?
C: We are very close now, especially since
I had the baby.
T: Do they know about your problems in
your marriage.
C: No. Like they can help me, right. ItÕs like my going to the pope for marriage advice. When I am ready to get divorced, if I
mean, then I will tell them - that they know all about.
After a few sessions she
admits that there is a man she is interested in at work. Although he is married too, he is also
dissatisfied with his relationship.
When I asked her what qualities attracted her to him she said the
following.
C: I like how self-sufficient he is. I mean he really knows how to take care
of himself. Not only is he the
main bread-winner of the family, but he is gourmet cook. If he is as good in the bedroom as he
is at work and in the kitchen, heÕd be perfect.
Carolyn rarely talks about
her child in therapy. I get the
sense that she doesnÕt spend much time with her, in that she leaves for work
early in the morning and she often spends evenings at her office. This may be in part due to her
attraction to her co-worker.
Assessment
Carolyn presents as
disengaged, self-protective, self-sufficient, sensitive to being controlled or
overly influenced by others.
When discussing her past
attachment relationships she presents few details, plays down negative
experiences and even presents contradictory information. She states that the stress of the
divorce was actually good for her in that they made her more independent. This is a common statement with people
who have a dismissing attachment status.
Carolyn constricts and plays
down her emotional experience.
When she speaks of her husband and the man she is attracted to, she
doesnÕt really refer to having emotional needs, but practical and sexual one.
Her answers tend to be short
and she doesnÕt offer the therapist much information about herself. This is
also common with people who have a dismissing attachment status.
Dismissing negative feelings
and experiences is a way of avoiding the pain associated with family attachment
experiences.
Engaging Carolyn into
therapy will be difficult because her childhood experiences has taught her that
safety is based on deactivating her attachment needs and feelings. To need therapy will require her to
admit that she canÕt deal with her problems on her own - a sign of weakness and
vulnerability. So the first treatment issue will be engagement and finding some
way of framing therapy that is not threatening to her defenses. With clients like Carolyn, going to
therapy to manage an obvious crisis or conflict may be a good as it gets.
Focusing initially on the practical aspects of therapy, skill building, is
helpful with clients like Carolyn.
Carolyn grew up in family
with distance, disengage parents - self-reliance may have been the best option
at the time. If she stays in therapy long enough, redirecting her attention to
her internal emotional experience will be key to psychological change. I would pay attention to when she might
be experiencing primary, background or social emotions that are communicated
nonverbally, and slowly and sensitively help her connect with those
emotions.
This tact is not going to be
very rewarding to the therapist. When you use your best sensitivity skills to
help her with identifying her emotionally needs sheÕll may just look at you and
say, ŅSo what?Ó But persistence is
key with this client. Years of
deactivating attachment needs is not going to change overnight. In fact, your sensitivity is likely to
cause her discomfort. He may
become so frightened that somebody sees her that she will begin to act out -
come late or miss sessions. A
combination of skill building, setting limits to acting out and persisting with
sensitive interpretation will hopefully pierce her protective defenses.
Vignette #3: Sandy
31-year old Jewish woman
In recovery (3 years) from
cocaine and alcohol dependency.
A survivor of child sexual
abuse.
Presents with a blunted
affect, introverted, insecure, analytical, cool and lifeless. She speaks with a
monotone voice and you find yourself asking her to repeat herself because she
speaks so softly.
Referred by probation for
attempting to stab her husband with a knife.
In the first session she
arrives 15 minutes late. She immediately wants to know my emergency
policy. She is concerned that
therapy brings up a lot of feelings for her and she wants to know my
availability between sessions. Her
previous therapist, whom she saw for three years about five years ago, was
available between sessions for emergencies.
(Immediately I am feeling
overwhelmed by her needs and pressure to ŅfixÓ her situation)
I discuss my policy of not
having 24-hour coverage and go over what services are available to her in the
county. I also suggest that
perhaps she may need to come in more than once a week if she begins to feel
overwhelmed. She says that she
canÕt afford to see you more than once a week and in fact, she was wondering if
I have a sliding scale. She says
that her former therapist saw her at a reduced rate. When I tell her that I donÕt reduce my fee, she gets a scowl
on her face and tells me that she thought it was unethical to not accommodate
peopleÕs financial situation and that she wasnÕt sure if she could continue in
therapy with me.
When asked about the
incident that resulted in her arrest she states that she and her husband had
just had sex when the telephone rang. It was his old girlfriend. She doesnÕt recall all the details but
she remembers getting angry and they started fighting. She doesnÕt remember how she got the
knife but she thought that she was going to kill herself, but she must have
started swinging the knife at her husband. Her daughter called the police.
She describes a long history
of short-term intimate relationships with both men and women that start off
very intense (sexually and emotionally) and then end abruptly. Sometimes she
angrily rejects her partner for no apparent reason. Other times she is rejected and falls apart. Her
relationship history is confusing and hard to follow. I find myself asking her clarifying questions. This pattern continues into her
discussion about her family of origin as well, when she disclosed that she was
sexually abused by her father.
When asked about her previous
therapy, she states that it mostly focused on her chemical addiction issues.
She states that she didnÕt go back to her previous therapist because she feels
that she outgrew the therapist.
When I follow up on this, it appears that she felt angry at her therapist
for disclosing too much information about herself.
I inquire about how her
sexual abuse was addressed in her previous therapy. She states that her previous therapist didnÕt really deal
with it because the focus of the therapy was her recovery. She explains that the philosophy of her
sponsor is to first get sober and then deal with family abuse issues. When I ask her if that is something she
would like to address in this therapy, there is a long silence, she looks up to
the ceiling and then says, ŅHe is dead now, you know my father, but he is
still inside of me.Ó When I ask
how so, she replies, ŅI donÕt know.Ó
Assessment: Sandy
Sandy has a mixture of
dismissing and preoccupied tendencies.
She angrily leaves relationships and is reluctant to come in more than
once a week (dismissing tendencies) and other times she is overwhelmed by
feelings of rejection, is wanting the therapist to take care of her by being
available for emergencies and reducing the fee (pre-occupied tendencies). Her discourse of her attachment
experiences is disjointed and dissociated in speech and mental processes. Sandy
shows some dissociative processes when asked about sexual abuse. Her story about the incident that got
her arrested suggests some dissociation as well.
SandyÕs attachment
experiences included trauma. States that she hasnÕt really worked on this issue
because recovery has been a priority. The incident of violence appears to be
more related to unresolved sexual trauma than substance abuse/dependency per
se. Some attachment researchers
and clinicians state that contrary to some preliminary findings suggesting that
preoccupied status is related to borderline personality disorder (BPD),
disorganization may be more related to this disorder. The characteristic oscillation between closeness and
distancing seen with persons suffering from BPD and the similar process seen
with disorganized attachment seems to make this hypothesis reasonable.
Sandy is disorganized
because she doesnÕt have a single strategy for dealing with separation anxiety
and reunion distress. She may oscillate between being helpless and needing
caretaking and being aggressive or distancing.
Treatment: Sandy
During the course of her
therapy, Sandy talked dispassionately about the sexual abuse by her
father. Though her stories were
extremely detailed (semantic memory), her descriptions seemed more like a
report or observation of someone else being abused. The challenge for her was to revisit those experiences but
in the retelling to include a sense of self (episodic memory) - which might
involve feelings or thoughts about what those experiences mean to her
life. The problem with Sandy is
that when she experiences emotion, she is quickly overwhelmed and moves into
rage states or dissociation (Remember what the question about her father did in
the first session?). So the
therapy will need to slowly address (through titration) these issues.
Vignette #4: Alison
44 years old (Irish decent)
Employed as a
psychotherapist
2 Children, son 23, daughter
20 (neither live at home)
Currently living with
husband who is employed as fireman.
Presents as insightful,
somewhat sarcastic and upset with husbandÕs Ņcontrolling and abusive
behaviors.Ó
A: I attended one of your workshops on
domestic violence and was very impressed with your knowledge of batterers and I
thought you could help me with my situation.
T: IÕll try.
A: Well, my husband and I have been
married for 28 years and from day one he has been controlling and abusive
towards me. He is always telling me
what to do, criticizing my cleaning, the way I decorate the house, my friends
and family, itÕs non-stop. We
fight all the time and if it wasnÕt for the fact that I am used to dealing with
people like him, IÕd be more of a wreak than I already am.
Alison
T: So you say/
A: /We have separated
numerous times over the course of our marriage, but we seem to always get back
together. I know I love him, but I
am not sure I can live with him.
T: Let me ask you a/
A: /I feel so embarrassed. My friends and colleagues see how
unhappy I am, but I just canÕt seem to leave him. He was a good father, and the sex, well that has never been
a problem. I think if I could get
him into therapy somehow, then maybe this relationship has a chance. What do you think?
Alison
T: Do I think you should
get him into therapy?
A: Yeah, I mean I donÕt
think he will ever go to therapy. HeÕs a fireman and all of his friends joke
with me about my work. They are so
self-absorbed with their masculinity - even if he did come itÕs doubtful that
heÕd get anything out of it. HeÕs
just like my father, who was the fire chief in the small town where I grew
up. He dominated and controlled my
poor little mother until it put her into an early grave. She died of a heart attack last yearÉ.
[starts to cry]. ItÕs still hard./
T: /I know./
Alison
A: /His drinking and
anger, itÕs unbearable. I got into
this fight with my sister at the funeral.
She was always on his side and thought that mom and I were a team. Of course, my sister will defend him till
she dies - she says my mother drove him crazy with her drinking, but I know for
a fact that the bastard drove her to the bottle. She and my father were always a team. There was no room for me in his life as
long as she was around. To this
day Nancy and mom, I, we canÕt really talk civilly to each other.
T: It sounds like you are
feeling a lot, about your relationship, your family and the loss of your
mother.
Alison
A: I just canÕt stand the verbal abuse any
longer. Maybe I should just bring him with me to our next session. You seem like you connect well with
men. What do you think?
T: Before we rush into anything, letÕs
take it slowly. IÕd like to get to
know more about you, your history.
There is a lot going on in your life - a difficult relationship,/
A: /Yes, your right./
T: /family problems and
A: ItÕs overwhelming./
T: /a significant loss./
A: /You sure know how to
get to the bottom line. I admire
that in a therapist./
Alison
T: /You must be feeling so much right
now. So before making any
decisions about couples therapy or not, maybe we should spend some time sorting
out all the thoughts and feelings you might be having about your situation.
A: IÕd like to see you
again this week. Is that possible?
T: Of course.
Assessment: Alison
Just from reading this text,
one can sense the anxiety in the room, which is an indicator of
preoccupation. Other signs include
her anger, her non-productive analysis of her relationships, her use of jargon
and psychobabble, and her not giving the therapist his conversational turn.
Her history suggests a weak
mother, who might have needed caretaking and a rejecting father. Again her narrative is angry and
critical, and the subject seems to be closed as to secure transcripts where
there is the ability to review the material with a fresh perspective.
There is an indicator of
unresolved loss.
Treatment: Alison
Preoccupied individuals use
proximity maintainence to regulate anxiety. Anger can be a way of staying connected even when the person
is not in close contact, or even alive as in this case. Treatment will focus on helping her
learn to regulate her anxiety in a more adaptive way. Affect regulation in this case will involve her learning to
access other emotions, learn to develop an appreciation for how prior
experiences will intensify current reactions to situations. When affect is so hightened, it is
difficult to put anotherÕs mind in your own, so Alison will need to develop
social cognition/empathy skills (activate her mirror neuron system). Additionally, Alison experiences a
great deal of negative affect. She
may need additional assistance learning how to achieve greater feelings of
wellbeing (altering relative Right/Left PFC activation) through mindfulness
training or meditation techniques.
Caution!!
As mentioned earlier in the
training, effective treatment of domestic violence cases will involve the
continual assessment of risk and the formulation of interventions geared to
reduce of the risk of future violence. Therefore, therapists working from an
attachment perspective (or any theoretical orientation for that matter) will
need to balance psychotherapeutic conceptualizations and interventions with the
continual assessment and treatment of violence and itÕs effects.
Earned Security
ŅI had a weak father,
domineering mother, contemptuous teachers, sadistic sergeants, destructive male
friendships, emasculating girlfriends, a wonderful wife, and three terrific
children. Where did I go right?Ó
– Jules Feiffer, illustrator and satirist
In longitudinal studies,
children assessed in the strange situation as infants are administered the AAI
as young adults to determine the continuity of attachment patterns over time
(Waters, Hamilton, and Weinfield, 2000). According to these studies there
is about an 80% continuity between infant attachment patterns and adult
attachment state of mind (Fraley, 2002). In 20% of the cases the
attachment status changes over time (usually from insecure to secure, but
sometimes the other way). The term Ņearned securityÓ is used for those
individuals who were either assessed in the strange situation as insecure and
later in life are assessed as secure, or whose experiences in childhood would
ordinarily lead us to expect an insecure state of mind (strange situation data
is not available) but are assessed as secure on the AAI (Roisman, Padron,
Sroufe and Egeland, 2002). This category of Ņearned secureÓ is
significant for clinicians, because it suggests that attachment status is
changeable. In other words, how a child or adult regulates attachment
distress can change over time. What factors contribute to earned
security? Researchers (Roisman, Padron, Sroufe and Egeland, 2002) have
found that when a child changes from insecure to secure, it is most likely to
be affected by a relationship. This makes sense because insecurity grows
out of relationships, so one would expect Ņearned securityÓ to grow out of
relationships.
Vignette #5: Luis
Luis is 24 year old, first
generation Mexican American.
He has been married for 3
years and has a 6 month old child.
His wife is 21 years old.
He works as manager of a
popular restaurant and is going to night school to become a chef.
He contacted you the morning
after a fight with his wife where he hit her with his elbow and caused a black
eye. You were able to see him that afternoon.
T: Can you tell me what
happened last night?
L: WeÕve been arguing a
lot about feeding the baby at night.
IÕm tired after working all day and going to school at night and I just canÕt focus at work
when I have to get up and feed the baby.
I know she is feeling tired too and she is might be thinking that I am
here complaining about her, but I know I play a role in this situation too.
T: So what happened last
night?
L: The baby was crying
and I heard him. I think I read
somewhere that you can let the baby cry for five minutes and sometimes they
will put themselves back to sleep - like itÕs just a false alarm.
Luis
T: I understand. We can
talk about that later, right now I am interested in what happened last night.
L: Well, she thought I
was sleeping, so she started pushing me to wake up. I just was waiting to see if the baby was going to stop
crying and so she kept pushing me harder and harder. I know she wasnÕt trying to hurt me, she just wanted me to
wake up because it was my turn to feed the baby. Anyhow, after about the fifth time, I just got angry and I
took my arm, with my elbow, I was sleeping with by back to her, and I just
swung it to tell her to stop pushing me.
Luis
T: What happened then?
L: She started crying
because I accidentally hit her in the eye. She got up and fed the baby and slept the rest of the night
in the babyÕs room.
T: You must have felt
pretty bad.
L: I swore that I would
never be like my father in that wayÉ(starts to get teary-eyed) I guess I was
feeling more upset and stressed out than I realized. But that is no excuse.
Luis
T: What do you mean you
swore that you wouldnÕt be like your father?
L: He used to beat my
mother and all us kids. What ever
belt he had on that day was the weapon of choice.
T: Why do you think he
acted that way?
L: I think it was his
upbringing. He was raised in
poverty and his parents beat him.
I mean, thatÕs no excuse and I think what he did was bad, but I
understand why he did it. Also,
having 9 kids and being the sole supporter didnÕt help either.
Luis
L: I used to think that
beating your wife and kids was normal.
No one ever talked about it so I just assumed it happened in everyoneÕs
family. I learned from my wife
that it doesnÕt have to be that way.
She had 10 brothers and sisters and each one felt loved and cared about.
T: What about your mom,
what was that relationship like?
L: She tried to be a good
mother, but I think she was pretty beaten down by him. She didnÕt have a lot of patience for
us. My older sister Rena was more
like a mother to me. She was so
loving. We are still very close
today.
T: Were their any other
people who stand out in your mind as having an effect on your life?
Luis
L: Definitely. I went to boarding school between ages
8 and 14. There was this English
teacher who I was very close to.
At first he and I would talk about school stuff, but then I began to
tell him problems. When I was
younger it was stuff about friends, but as I got older heÕd help me with
feelings I was having about girls.
I could never talk to my father about anything and my mother would just
say things like, ŅJust do your school work and donÕt think about silly
things.Ó But he was, I could talk
to him about anything. It seemed
like anything I said was important.
It felt good. I was sorry
that I left the school.
T: What do you hope to
get out of therapy?
Luis
L: Well, IÕve never been
to a therapist before. As I think
about it I am not sure how you can help me. Wait a minute, let me seeÉ. Well, I guess I need help with
my anger and stress. I think I
have tried real hard not to be like my father, but as I think about it now, I
think itÕs going to take more than just trying not to be like him.
T: So are you saying that
you donÕt want to be like your father?
L: No, not exactly. I am saying that I donÕt want to be
like him in that way. He had good
qualities too,like he was a hard worker.
But sometimes itÕs easier to just remember the bad times.
Luis
T: Luis, you mentioned
earlier that you are stressed out lately.
Can you tell me more about that?
L: Well, with work and school, and now the
baby, IÕm just tired a lot, moody and there isnÕt time for anything fun.
T: Has this been just
since you have been in school and the baby?
L: Well, my wife says
that I tend to be a little depressed at times.
T: Do you think this is
true?
L: Maybe, I donÕt really
know.
Assessment: Luis
Luis most probably will have
an earned-secure AAI. He was
physically abused and witnessed violence as a child. He mentioned two important relationships, his older sister
and teacher, both seemed to provide a secure base for him to develop many of
the capacities of secure attachment:
his ability to reflect on himself and on the mind of others (his
wife). You get the sense that he
is thinking as the interview progressed and not just using canned speech or
jargon. He was even autonomous
enough to disagree with or clarify his thoughts with the interviewer.
Treatment: Luis
Luis will certainly be
easier to work with than our other examples. He is motivated, self-reflective
and is able to put himself into the mind of others. He has a balanced perspective on his childhood, but
nevertheless realizes he has some work to do if he doesnÕt want to repeat the
violence of his father. There is
some suggestion of depression but this needs further follow-up. The work with Luis will follow the same
protocol that Bowlby laid out, and continuing to focus on developing the same
capacities of secure attachment (capacities of the PFC).
References
and Suggested Reading
Ainsworth, M. D.
S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the
strange situation. Hillsdale, N.J.:
Erlbaum.
Bartholomew, K. & Horowitz, L.M. (1991) Attachment styles among young adults: A test of a
four category model. Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 61 (2), 226 – 244.
Bartholomew, K.; & Moretti, M.
(2002) The dynamics of measuring attachment. Attachment
and Human Development, 4(2),
162-165.
Bowlby, J. (1969) Attachment , Vol. 1 of Attachment and loss. London: Hogarth
Press. New York: Basic Books;
Harmondsworth: Penguin (1971)
Bowlby, J. (1973) , Separation: Anxiety & Anger. Vol. 2 of Attachment and loss London: Hogarth Press;
New York: Basic Books; Harmondsworth: Penguin (1975).
Bowlby, J. (1980)
Loss: Sadness & Depression, in Vol. 3 of Attachment and loss, London: Hogarth Press. New York: Basic Books;
Harmondsworth: Penguin (1981).
Bowlby, J. (1988).
A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London: Routledge.
Brassard, M.R.; Germain, R.; and Hart, S.N. (1987). Psychological
maltreatment of children and youth.
New York: Pergamon Press.
Brennan, K. A.; Clark, C. L. and Shaver, P. R.
(1998). Self-report measurment of
adult attachment: An integrative overview. In J.A. Simpson and W.S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory
and close relationships (pp. 46-76),
New York: Guilford Press.
Bretherton, Inge (1992). The Origins of
Attachment Theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth. Developmental Psychology.
Vol. 28 (5) September, pp. 759-775.
Cassidy J. & P. R. Shaver (Eds.)(1999), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and
clinical applications. New York: Guilford
Press.
Cowan, C and
Cowan, P. (1999). When Partners
Become Parents: The Big Life Change for Couples. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Damasio, A. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens: Body
and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness.
New York: Harcourt Brace.
Damasio, A.
(2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, Sorrow, and
the Feeling Brain. New York: Harcourt.
Davidson, Richard (2000). Affect style,
psychopathology and resilience: Brain mechanisms and plasticity. American
Psychologist, 55 (11), 1196-1214.
Davidson, Richard (2003). Darwin
and the Neural Bases of Emotion and Affective Style. Annals N.Y. Acad.
Sciences. 1000: 316–336 (2003).
Davidson, Richard (2004). What does
the prefrontal cortex ŅdoÓ in affect: Perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry
research. Biological Psychology, 67, 219-233.
DiClemente, C.C. & Hughes, S.O. Stages of change profiles in outpatient
alcoholism treatment. Journal
of Subsatnce Abuse, 2, 217-235 (1990).
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V.
(1978). The facial action coding system. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologists Press.
Fonagy, P. (2001) Attachment Theory and Psychoanalysis. New York: Other Press
Fonagy, P., &
Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in
self-organization. Development and Psychopathology, 9:677-699.
Fonagy, P.; Target, M.; Gergely, G. and Jurist, E.J. (2002) Affect Regulation,
Mentalization and the Development of the Self. New York: Other Press.
Fraley R. Chris and Shaver,
Phillip R. (2000). Adult Romantic
Attachment: Theoretical Developments, Emerging Controversies, and Unanswered
Questions. Review of General Psychology, Vol. 4, No. 2, 132-154.
Fraley, R. Chris (2002).
Attachment Stability From Infancy to Adulthood: Meta-Analysis and Dynamic
Modeling of Developmental Mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review.
Vol. 6, No. 2, 123–151.
George, C. & West, M. (2001). The Development
and Preliminary Validation of a New Measure of Adult Attachment: The Adult
Attachment Projective. Attachment and Human Development. 3, 30-61.
George, C., Kaplan, N. & Main, M. (1996). Adult
Attachment Interview. (Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology,
University of California, Berkeley (3rd ed.).
Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love as
conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52, 511-524.
Hazan, Cindy
and Zeifman, Debra (1999). Pair
bonds as attachments. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 336—354). New York: Guilford Press.
Henderson, A.J.Z., Bartholomew,
K, and Dutton, D.G. (1997) He
loves me, He loves me not: Attachment and separation resolution of abused
women. Journal of Family Violence
12 (2), 169 - 192.
Hesse, E, Main, M.,
Yost-Abrams, K., Rifkin, A. (2003).
Unresolved states regarding loss or abuse have "second
generation" effects: Disorganization, role-inversion, and frightening ideation in the off-spring
of traumatized, non-maltreated
parents. In: Solomon M, Siegel DJ, eds. Healing trauma. New York: Norton.
Hesse, Erik (1999). The adult attachment interview:
Historical and current perspectives. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 395—433). New York: Guilford Press.
Hesse, Erik; Main, Mary
(2000). Disorganized infant,
child, and adult attachment: Collapse in behavioral and attentional
strategies. ; Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, Vol
48(4). pp. 1097-1127.
Holmes, J.
(2001). In search of the secure
base. London: Routledge.
Horvath, A.O. and
Luborsky, L (1993). The Role of the Therapeutic Alliance in Psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 61, No. 4, 561-573.
Iacoboni,
M., Woods, R.P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J.C. and Rizzolatti, G.
(1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528.
Johnson, Susan
(1996). The Practice of Emotionally Focused Marital Therapy: Creating Connection. N.Y.:
Brunner and Mazel.
Johnson, Susan
(2002). Emotionally Focused Couple Therapy with Trauma Survivors:
Strengthening Attachment Bonds. N.Y.:
Guilford
Kandel, Eric R. (1999). Biology and the Future of
Psychoanalysis: A New Intellectual Framework for Psychiatry Revisited. American
Journal of Psychiatry 1999; 156:505–524.
Ligiego, D. P.;
Gelso, C J.. Countertransference, attachment, and the working alliance: The therapists contributions. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research,
Practice, Training. Vol. 39(1) Spring 2002. pp. 3-11.
Liotti, G. (1999b). Understanding the dissociative
processes: The contribution of attachment theory. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 19, 757-783.
Main M., Goldwyn R. (1993).
Adult Attachment Classification System. Unpublished manuscript,
University of California, Berkeley.
Main, M. & Solomon, J.
(1986) Discovery of
an insecure disorganized / disoriented attachment pattern: procedures, findings
and implications for classification of behaviour. In M. Yogman
& T. B.
Brazelton, Affective development in infancy. Norwood (pages 95-124), NJ: Ablex.
Marmaras,
Elizabeth; Lee, Sandra S.; Siegel, Harold; The Relationship Between Attachment
Styles and Vicarious Traumatization in Female Trauma Therapists. Journal
of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, Vol 26(1), 2003. Special
issue: Traumatic Stress and Its Aftermath: Cultural, Community, and
Professional Contexts. pp. 81-92.
Meyer, Bjrn;
Pilkonis, Paul A.. Attachment Style. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice,
Training. Vol. 38(4) Winter 2001.
Mikulincer, M.,
Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of the attachment system in
adulthood: Threat-related primes increase the accessibility of mental
representations of attachment figures. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 83, 881-895
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P.R. & Pereg, D. (2003) Attachment Theory and Affect Regulation: The Dynamics,
Development, and Cognitive Consequences of Attachment-Related Strategies.
Motivation and Emotion 27(2): 77-102.
Milkulincer, M., & Shaver, P. (2001). Attachment theory and intergroup bias:
Evidence that priming the secure base schema attenuates negative reactions to
out-groups. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
97-115.
Morgan, Hillary and Shaver, Phillip R. (1999). Attachment
Processes and Commitment to Romantic Relationships. In Adams, J. and Jones, W.
(eds), Handbook of Interpersonal Commitment and Relationship Stability, New
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Panksepp, J (1998). Affective
neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions, New York: Oxford
Univ. Press.
Prochaska, J.O.,
DiClemente, C.C., & Norcross, C.C. (1992). In search of how people change:
Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist, 47, 1102-1127.
Roisman, Glen, Padron, Elena, Sroufe, L. Alan and Egeland, Byron (2002). Earned-Secure Attachment Status in Retrospect and Prospect. Child Development. July/August.
Rubino, G.,
Barker, C., Roth, T. & Fearon, P. (2000). Therapist empathy and depth of
interpretation in response to potential alliance ruptures: The role of
therapist and patient attachment styles. Psychotherapy Research, 10, 408-420.
Rutgers, A.H.,
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzendoorn, M.H., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes,
I.A. (2004). Autism and attachment: A meta-analytic review. Child Psychol
Psychiatry. 2004 Sep; 45(6):1123-34.
Ryff, Carol D., Singer, Burton H and Love, Gayle Dienberg ( 2004). Positive health: Connecting
well-being with biology. Phil.
Trans. R. Soc. London Bulletin, 359,1383–1394.
Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect dysrgulation and disorders of the self. New York: Norton.
Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. New York: Norton.
Schore, A.N.
(1994). Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of
Emotional Development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Shaver, P. R., & Clark, C. L. (1994). The
psychodynamics of adult romantic attachment. In J. M. Masling & R. F.
Bornstein (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on object relations theories (pp. 105-156). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Shaver, P.; Belsky, J.; & Brennan, K.
(2000) The Adult Attachment Interview and self-reports of romantic attachment:
Associations across domains and methods. Personal
Relationships, 7, 25-43.
Shaver, Phillip & Mikulincer, Mario (2002). Attachment-Related Psychodynamics.
Attachment and Human
Development, 2002, 4: 133-161.
Shaver, Phillip R., Belsky,
Jay, and Brennan, Kelly (2000). The
adult attachment interview and self-reports of romantic attachment:
Associations across domains and methods.
Personal Relationships, 7, 25-43.
Siegel DJ (2001).
Toward an Interpersonal Neurobiology of the Developing Mind: Attachment
relationships, Ņmindsight,Ó and neural integration. Infant Mental Health
Journal, Special Edition on Contributions of the Decade of the Brain to Infant
Psychiatry, 22:67-94.
Siegel, D. J. and Hartzell, M. (2003). Parenting
from the Inside Out: How a Deeper Self-Understanding Can Help You Raise
Children Who Thrive. New York: Tarcher/Putnan.
Siegel, DJ (1999). The developing mind: How
relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are. New York: Guilford Press.
Slade, A. (1999). Attachment theory and research:
Implications for the theory and practice of individual psychotherapy for
adults.(In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment:
Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 575—594). New York: Guilford Press.).
Slade, Arietta
(2002) Keeping the baby in mind: Critical factor in perinatal mental health.
ZERO TO THREE, June/July 2002.
Solomon & Siegel (Eds.) (2003). Healing Trauma:
attachment, mind, body, and brain. New York: Norton.
Sonkin, Daniel (2005). Attachment
Theory and Psychotherapy. The Therapist, a publication of the California Association of Marriage and Family Therapists. January/February, 2005.
Sonkin, D.J.
(1998). Wounded men: A guide for healing from childhood abuse for men and
their partners. New York: Adams
Publications.
Sonkin, Daniel and Dutton, Don (in press). Attachment theory and domestic
violence. In Dutton, Don and
Sonkin, Daniel (ed). Treatment
of Intimate Violence: Multidimensional Perspectives on Addressing Violence in
Psychotherapy. New York: Haworth Publishing.
Sroufe L.A.
(1995). Emotional Development: The Organization of Emotional Life in the
Early Years. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Sroufe, L. A., Duggal, S., Weinfield, N. and Carlson, E.
(2000). Relationships, development
and psychopathology. In Sameroff,
A., Lewis, M. and Miller, S. (eds), Handbook of Developmental Psychology,
Second Edication. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Publishers.
Stern, D.
(1998). The process of therapeutic
change involving implicit knowledge: Some implications of developmental
observations for adult psychotherapy. Infant Mental Health Journal, Volume
(19), 300-308.
Stern, Daniel (1985). The
Interpersonal World of the Infant Basic
Books.
Stern, Daniel
(2004). The Present Moment in
Psychotherapy and Everyday Life. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
Tronick, E.
(1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American
Psychologist, 44, 112–119.
Tulving E (1993). Varieties of
consciousness and levels of awareness in memory. In: Attention, Selection,
Awareness, and Control: A Tribute to Donald Broadbent,
Baddeley A, Weiskrantz L, eds. London: Oxford University Press, pp 283-299.
Tyrrell, C. L.,
Dozier, M., Teague, G. B. & Fallot, R. D. (1999). Effective treatment
relationships for persons with serious psychiatric disorders: The importance of attachment states of mind.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
67, 725-733.
Urry,
H.L., Nitschke, J.B., Dolski, I., Jackson, D.C., Dalton, K.M., Mueller, C.J.,
Rosenkranz, M.A., Ryff, C.D., Singer, B.H. and Davidson, R.J. (2004).
Making a life worth living: Neural correlates of well-being. Psychological
Science, 15, 367–372.
Van
IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995b). Adult attachment representations,
parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A meta-analysis on the
predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview." Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 387-403.
Van IJzendoorn, M.
H., and Sagi, A. (1999). Cross-cultural patterns of attachment: Universal and
contextual dimensions. In J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook
of attachment (pp. 713-734). New York:
Guilford.
Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Juffer, F., & Duyvesteyn,
M. G. C. (1995). Breaking the intergenerational cycle of insecure attachment -
A review of the effects of attachment-based interventions on maternal
sensitivity and infant security. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
and Allied Disciplines, 36, 225-248.
Vaughan, S.
(1997). The talking cure: Why traditional
talking therapy offers a better chance for long-term relief than any drug. New
York: Owl Books.
Waters E.,
Merrick, S., Treboux, D, Crowell, J., and Albersheim, L. (2000). Attachment
Security in Infancy and Early Adulthood: A Twenty-Year Longitudinal Study. Child
Development, May/June 2000, Volume 71, Number 3, Pages 684-689.
West, M., & George, C. (1999). Violence in intimate adult relationships: An attachment
theory perspective. Attachment and Human Development, 1, 137-156.